multiple baseline design disadvantages

Back to Blog

multiple baseline design disadvantages

Multiple baseline and multiple probe designs. Although publication dates would suggest that Kazdin and Kopel (1975) was published before Hersen and Barlow (1976), Kazdin and Kopel cite Hersen and Barlow, and not the other way around. It would be an even greater concern if the treatment were an instructional program that requires several weeks or months to implement. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(75)80181-X, Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, W. R. (2013). In the end, judgments about the plausibility of threats and number of tiers needed must be made by researchers, editors, and critical readers of research. They argue that because nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs lack an across-tier comparison in real time (the criticism described above), they cannot verify the prediction of the behavior pattern in the absences of intervention. This insensitivity is not due to poor experimental design or implementation, it is built in to the nature of multiple baseline designs across participants. In addition, functionally isolating tiers (e.g., across settings) such that they are highly unlikely to be subjected to the same instances of a threat can also contribute to this goal. Second, the across-tier comparison assumes that extraneous variables will affect multiple tiers similarly. Barlow, D. H., Nock, M. K., & Hersen, M. (2009). Reasons for these specifications will become clear later in the article.) Independent from Watson and Workman (1981), Hayes (1981) published a lengthy article introducing SCDs to clinical psychologists and made the point that these designs are well-suited to conducting research in clinical practice. The tutorial begins with instructions for how to create a simple multiple condition/phase (e.g., withdrawal research design) line graph. This assumption was initially identified by Kazdin and Kopel in 1975, but its implications for the rigor of the across-tier comparison have rarely been discussed since that time. Potential setting-level events include staffing changes in classroom, redecoration or renovation of the physical environment, and changes in the composition of the peer group in a classroom, group home, or worksite. On the other hand, across-tier comparisons may be strengthened by arranging tiers to be as similar as possible so that they would be more likely to be exposed to the same coincidental events. A baseline (A) and an intervention (B) are included in a straightforward AB design psychological experiment (B). Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change (3rd ed.). This critical requirement is mainly addressed by the lag between phase changes in successive phases. If the pattern of change shortly after implementation of the treatment is replicated in the other tiers after differing lengths of time in baseline (i.e., different amounts of maturation), maturation becomes increasingly implausible as an alternative explanation. The author has no known conflicts of interest to disclose. As we argued above, the observation of no change in an untreated tier is not strong evidence against a coincidental event affecting the treated tier. This has been the sharpest point of criticism of nonconcurrent multiple baselines. The withdrawal phase of an A-B-A design is important because it shows that the results of the intervention weren't just a result of a difference in time. Kazdin, A. E., & Kopel, S. A. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. The nature of control for coincidental events (i.e., history) provided by the within-tier comparison in both concurrent and nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs is relatively straightforward. Part of Springer Nature. On the other hand, if we see a change in a treated tier and no change in untreated tiers, does this constitute strong evidence to rule out threats to internal validity? in their classic 1968 article that defined applied behavior analysis. On the other hand, if we observe that one tier shows a change whereas other tiers that have been observed for similar amounts of time do not show similar changes, this may reduce the plausibility of the maturation threat. A researcher who puts great confidence in the across-tier comparison could falsely reject the idea that coincidental events were the cause of observed effects. 288335). Watson and Workman did not explicitly address threats to internal validity other than coincidental events. Estimating reliabilities and correcting for sampling error in indices of within-person dynamics derived from intensive longitudinal data, Optimizing Detection of True Within-Person Effects for Intensive Measurement Designs: A Comparison of Multilevel SEM and Unit-Weighted Scale Scores, https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBE.0000044735.51022.5d, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.49.2.193, https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(75)80181-X, https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-020-00263-x, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0111-y, https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0, http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, SI: Commentary on Slocum et al, Threats to Internal Validity. Behavioral cusps: A developmental and pragmatic concept for behavior analysis. If an effective treatment were to have a broad impact on multiple tiers, the logic of the design would be to falsely attribute these effects to possible extraneous variables. Consequently, it is often difficult or impossible to dismiss rival hypotheses or explanations. These views of multiple baseline designs have been carried through into much of the single-case methodological literature and textbooks to the current day. Webmultiple baseline (3 forms) 1. across bx 2. across settings, 3. across subjects or groups using 3-5 tiers. For example, instrumentation is addressed primarily through observer training, calibration, and IOA. We are not pointing to flaws in execution of the design; we are pointing to inherent weaknesses. Likewise, setting-level coincidental events are those that contact a single setting. We examine how these comparisons address maturation, testing and session experience, and coincidental events. In the past, there was significant controversy regarding the relative rigor of concurrent and nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs. Pergamon. If a potential treatment effect is seen in one tier, the researcher cannot refer to data from the same day in an untreated tier because the tiers are not synchronized in real time and may not even overlap in real time. Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). In this article, we argue that the primary reliance on across-tier comparisons and the resulting deprecation of nonconcurrent designs are not well-justified. These coincidental events would contact all tiers of a multiple baseline that include this individual participant, but not tiers that do not involve this participant. However, an across-tier comparison is not definitive because testing or session experience could affect the tiers differently. By nature, undetected events are unknown. They state, the nonconcurrent multiple baseline across participants design is inherently weaker than other multiple baseline design variations. . After implementing the treatment for the first tier, they say, rather than reversing the just produced change, he instead applies the experimental variable to one of the other as yet unchanged responses. Maturational changes may be smooth and gradual, or they may be sudden and uneven. However, we can never ensure that any two contexts or any two session times are not subject to unique events during the study. Under the proposed definition, such a study would not be considered a full-fledged multiple baseline. WebDisadvantages to Multiple Baseline Designs -Weaker method of showing experimental control than a reversal (b/c no withdrawal of treatment) -Delay in treatment can occur as The logic of replicated within-tier analysis applies equally to concurrent and nonconcurrent designs. If we observe a potential treatment effect in one tier and corresponding changes in untreated tiers after similar amounts of time (i.e., number of days), maturation becomes a more plausible alternative explanation of the initial potential treatment effect. Multiple baseline designs are intended to evaluate whether there is a functional (causal) relation between the introduction of the independent variable and changes in the dependent variable. In the current study, it is likely that exposure to some of the measures can affect scores on other measures or repeated exposure to a measure can lead to socially desirable responding or Finally, we make recommendations for more rigorous use, reporting, and evaluation of multiple baseline designs. While the fact that the researcher does not use a large number of participants has its advantages, it also has a downside: Because the experimental trials are run on only one subject, it is difficult to empirically show with the experiment's data that the findings will generalize out to larger populations. This skepticism of nonconcurrent designs stems from an emphasis on the importance of across-tier comparisons and relatively low importance placed on replicated within-tier comparisons for addressing threats to internal validity and establishing experimental control. Without the latter you cannot conclude, with confidence, that the intervention alone is responsible for observed behavior changes since baseline (or probe) data are not concurrently collected on all tiers from the start of the investigation. If either of these assumptions are not valid for a coincidental event, then the presence and function of that event would not be revealed by the across-tier analysis. Examples could include family events, illness, changed social interactions (e.g., breaking up with a partner), losing or gaining access to a social service program, etc. 66 : Discuss the advantages and disadvantages of using visual inspection of graphs rather than statistics to evaluate the significance of the results. Pearson Education. Throughout this article we have referred to the importance of replicating within-tier comparisons, emphasizing the idea that tiers must be arranged with sufficient lag in phase changes so that specific threats to internal validity are logically ruled out. All three of these dimensions of lag are necessary to rigorously control for commonly recognized threats to internal validity and establish experimental control. Hersen, M., & Barlow, D. H. (1976). In the case of multiple baseline designs, a stable baseline supports a strong prediction that the data path would continue on the same trajectory in the absence of an effective treatment; these predictions are said to be verified by observing no change in trajectories of data in other tiers that are not subjected to treatment; and replication is demonstrated when a treatment effect is seen in multiple tiers. Likewise, in a multiple baseline across settings, selecting settings that tend to share extraneous events would make the across-tier analysis more powerful than would selecting settings that share few common events. If A changes after B is put into practice, a researcher can draw the Conclusion that B caused A to change. These reports do not provide the information necessary to rigorously evaluate maturation or coincidental events. In this highly influential early textbook on SCD, Hersen and Barlow describe only the across-tier analysis and fail to mention replicated within-tier comparisons. . Rather, the passage of time allows for more opportunities for participants to interact with their environmentleading to maturational changes. This is a significant problem for the across-tier comparison because its logic is dependent on these two assumptions. Given this dilemma, priority should be given to optimizing the within-tier comparisons because this is the comparison that can confer stronger control. An important drawback of pre-experimental designs is that they are subject to numerous threats to their validity. Basic Books. The process begins with a simple baseline-treatment (AB) comparisona change from baseline to treatment within a single tier. When changes in data occur immediately after the phase change, are large in magnitude, and are consistent across tiers, threats to internal validity tend to be less plausible explanations of the data patterns, and fewer tiers would be required to rule them out. Although many maturational changes are gradual, more sudden changes are possible. WebIn yet a third version of the multiple-baseline design, multiple baselines are established for the same participant but in different settings. It is interesting that this emphasis on across-tier comparisons is the opposite of that evident in Baer et al. On resolving ambiguities of the multiple-baseline design: Problems and recommendations. Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Learn more about Institutional subscriptions. In such an instance, there may be a disruption to experimental control in only one-tier of the design and not others, thus influencing the degree of internal It is possible that a coincidental event may be present for all tiers but have different effects on different tiers. If a potential treatment effect is seen in one tier and on the same day there is no change in other tiers, this is taken as strong evidence that the potential treatment effect was not a result of a coincidental event, because a coincidental event would have had an effect on all tiers. If the baseline phase provides sufficiently stable data to support a strong prediction of the subsequent data path and the data path prediction is contradicted by the actual data after the introduction of the independent variable, this provides some suggestion that the independent variable may have been the cause of the changea potential treatment effect. Three phonological patterns were targeted for each child. To answer the first question, the one must distinguish signal (systematic change) from noise (unsystematic variance). Hersen and Barlows (1976) textbook appears to be the first complete description of the multiple baseline design with many of the ideas about experimental control that are current to this day. As a result, concurrent and nonconcurrent designs are virtually identical in their control for maturation threats. Oxford University Press. Type I Errors and Power in Multiple Baseline Designs, Assessing consistency of effects when applying multilevel models to single-case data. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). (1973). This control assumes that the replications are sufficiently offset in real time (e.g., calendar days) to ensure that a single coincidental event could not plausibly cause the effects observed in multiple tiers. Type I errors and power in multiple baseline designs. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 43, 605616. The authors discuss two designs commonly used to demonstrate reliable control of an important behavior change (p. 94). - 181.212.136.34. The dimension of time is recognized in the requirement that phase changes be lagged in real timethat is, the date on which the phase changes are made. Houghton Mifflin. However, if this within-tier pattern is replicated in multiple tiers after differing numbers of baseline sessions, this threat becomes increasingly implausible. (p. 365), Of course, the major problem with this [nonconcurrent multiple baseline] strategy is that the control for history (i.e., the ability to assess subjects concurrently) is greatly diminished. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Webtreatment (Kazdin & Nock, 2003). Use the Previous and Next buttons to navigate the slides or the slide controller buttons at the end to navigate through each slide. With stable data, the range within which future data points will fall is PubMed Later they present an overall evaluation of the strength of multiple baseline designs, attributing its primary weakness to its reliance on the across-tier comparison, The multiple baseline design is considerably weaker than the withdrawal design as the controlling effects of the treatment on each of the target behaviors is not directly demonstrated . Coincidental events (i.e., history) are specific events that occur at a particular time (or across a particular period) and could cause changes in behavior. Thus, to the degree that nonconcurrent designs support longer lags between phases changes than concurrent designs, they may support stronger control of the threat of coincidental events through replicated within-tier comparisons. WebWhat are some disadvantages of alternating treatment design? In a review of the SCD literature, Shadish and Sullivan (2011) found multiple baseline designs making up 79% of the SCD literature (54% multiple baseline alone, 25% mixed/combined designs). Further, if the potential treatment effect is more gradual (as one might expect from an educational intervention on a complex skill), maturational changes may be impossible to distinguish from treatment effects. For example, phase changes in two consecutive tiers may be lagged by three sessions, but if one to three sessions are conducted per day, the baseline phases could include the same number of days (problem for controlling maturation) and the phase change could occur on the same day in both tiers (problem for controlling coincidental events). The multiple baseline family of designs includes multiple baseline and multiple probe designs. The current SCD methodological literature and most SCD textbooks claim that because the tiers of nonconcurrent multiple baseline are not synchronized in real time they have a diminished capacity to control for extraneous variables, in particular coincidental events (e.g., Carr, 2005; Gast et al., 2018; Harvey et al., 2004; Johnston et al., 2020). Textbook authors, editors, and readers of research should consider nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs to be capable of supporting conclusions every bit as strong as those from concurrent designs. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Coincidental events might be expected to be more variable in their effect than interventions that are designed to have consistent effects. Behavioral Interventions, 33(2), 160172. Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. et al. Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Part of Springer Nature. A potential treatment effect in any single tier could plausibly be explained as a result of a coincidental event. B. Third, patterns of results influence the number of tiers needed to yield definitive conclusions. Threats to Internal Validity in Multiple-Baseline Design Variations, https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-022-00326-1, Concurrence on Nonconcurrence in Multiple-Baseline Designs: A Commentary on Slocum et al. That is, experimental control has not been convincingly demonstrated. WebGive two advantages and two disadvantages of quasi-experimental designs. Elapsed time does not directly cause maturational changes in behavior. These baseline-treatment comparisons, which we will refer to as tiers, differ from one another with respect to participants, behaviors, settings, stimulus materials, and/or other variables. A coincidental event may contact a single unit of analysis (e.g., one of four participants) or multiple units (e.g., all participants). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 533544. The within-tier analysis seeks replication of these potential treatment effects in additional tiers of the design. Further, for the across-tier comparison to detect the influence of a coincidental event, that event must not only contact multiple tiers, it must cause similar changes in the dependent measure across multiple tiers. We use function of elapsed time descriptively rather than causally. These could include presence of observers, testing procedures, exposure to testing stimuli, attention from implementers, being removed from the typical setting, exposure to a special setting, and so on. If these assumptions are not valid, then it would be possible to observe stable baselines in untreated tiers even though the change in the treated tier was a result of an extraneous variable. Textbooks commonly describe and characterize the design without clearly defining it. (p. 206). (p. 325), Compared to its concurrent multiple baseline design sibling, a non-concurrent arrangement is inherently weaker . . The details of situations in which this across-tier comparison is valid for ruling out threats to internal validity are more complex than they may appear. In both forms of multiple baseline designs, a potential treatment effect in the first tier would be vulnerable to the threat that the changes in data could be a result of testing or session experience. (2018) state: Confidence that maturation and history [coincidental events] threats are under control is based on observing (a) an immediate change in the dependent variable upon introduction of the independent variable, and (b) baseline (or probe) condition levels remaining stable while other tiers are exposed to the intervention. The across-tier analysis of coincidental events is the main way that concurrent and nonconcurrent multiple baselines differ. ), Single case research methodology: Applications in special education and behavioral sciences (pp. Additionally, the The vast majority of contemporary published multiple baseline designs describe the timing of phases in terms of sessions rather than days or dates. Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Research methodologists have identified numerous potential alternative explanations that are threats to internal validity (e.g., Campbell & Stanley, 1963; Cooper et al., 2020; Kazdin, 2021; Shadish et al., 2002). For example, knowing the date of session 10 in tier 1 tells us nothing about the date of session 10 in tier 2. Journal of Behavioral Education, 13(4), 213226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932512452794, Lanovaz, M. J., & Turgeon, S. (2020). As we mentioned above, across-tier comparisons require the assumptions that coincidental events will (1) contact and (2) have similar effects on all tiers of the design. Thus, the additional temporal separation that is possible in a nonconcurrent design is a strength rather than a weakness in controlling for coincidental events. a potential treatment effect in the first tier would be vulnerable to the threat that the changes in data could be a result of In particular, within-tier comparisons may be strengthened by isolating tiers from one another in ways that reduce the chance that any single coincidental event could coincide with a phase change in more than one tier (e.g., temporal separation). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings (3rd ed.). A broad and general impression such as these designs are relatively strong is not sufficient to guide experimental design decisions or to evaluate particular variations of multiple baseline designs. Data from the treatment phase in one tier can be compared to corresponding baseline data in another tier. Behavior Therapy, 6(5), 601608. Thus, both of the articles introducing nonconcurrent multiple baselines made explicit arguments that replicated within-tier comparisons are sufficient to address the threat of coincidental events. PubMed Central Second, in a remarkably understated reference to the across-tier comparison, Baer et al. Kazdin, A. E. (2021). An example of multiple baseline across behaviors might be to use feedback to develop a comprehensive exercise program that involves stretching, aerobic exercise, By synchronized we mean that session 1 in all tiers takes place before session 2 in any tier, and this ordinal invariance of session number across tiers is true for all sessions. Predi Abab Design Essay If it changes at that point, evidence is accruing that the experimental variable is indeed effective, and that the prior change was not simply a matter of coincidence (p. 94). Slider with three articles shown per slide. Finally, practitioners whose work may be influenced by SCD research must understand these issues so they can give appropriate weight to research findings. This would align the definition with the critical features required to demonstrate experimental control and thereby allow strong causal statements based on multiple baseline designs. This has at least two effects: first, the multiple baseline is seen as weaker than the withdrawal design because of this dependence on the across-tier analysis; and second, when nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs are introduced years later, their rigor will be understood by many methodologists in terms of control by across-tier comparisons only, without consideration of replicated within-tier comparisons. The first is the reversal design and the authors describe the important applied limitation with this designsituations in which reversals are not possible or feasible in applied settings. Further, it is impossible to know how many events, which events, or the severity of the events that are missed by an across-tier comparison. For both types of comparisons, addressing maturation begins with an AB contrast in a single tier. The time lag must be sufficiently long so that no single event could produce potential treatment effects in more than one tier. It is surprising that there is no single consensus definition of multiple baseline designs. We have no known conflict of interest to disclose. This paper describes procedures for using these designs, Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Controlling for maturation requires baseline phases of distinctly different temporal durations (i.e., number of days); controlling for testing and session experience requires baseline phases of substantially different number of sessions; and controlling for coincidental events requires phase changes on sufficiently offset calendar dates. If session experience exerted a small degree of influence on the DV, an effect might be observed in settings where the behavior is more likely, but not in settings where the behavior is less likely. Oxford. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2020). Behavior Modification, 40(6), 852873. A : true B : false. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203, Johnston, J. M., Pennypacker, H. S., & Green, G. (2020). To offer some guidance, we believe that under ideal conditionsadequate lags between phase changes, circumstances that do not suggest that threats are particularly likely, and clear results across tiersthree tiers in a multiple baseline can provide strong control against threats to internal validity. Nonconcurrent designs are said to be substantially compromised with respect to internal validity and in general this limitation is ascribed to their supposed weakness in addressing threats of coincidental events (i.e., history).

Bugle Instrument For Sale, Hospitality Sales And Marketing Ppt, Did Johnny Mathis Rebuild His House, Iberostar Paraiso Lindo Vs Maya, Active Warrants List Herkimer County, Ny, Articles M

multiple baseline design disadvantages

multiple baseline design disadvantages

Back to Blog